Organizational Psychology Research on Work Extrinsic and Intrinsic Motivation Scale

Description
The Work Extrinsic and Intrinsic Motivation Scale (WEIMS) is an 18-item measure of work motivation theoretically grounded in self-determination theory (Deci & Ryan, 2000). The purpose of the present research was twofold. First, the applicability of the WEIMS in different work environments was evaluated. Second, its factorial structure and psychometric properties were assessed.

Work Extrinsic and Intrinsic Motivation Scale: Its Value for Organizational
Psychology Research
Maxime A. Tremblay, Ce´line M. Blanchard,
Sara Taylor, and Luc G. Pelletier
University of Ottawa
Martin Villeneuve
Department of National Defence, Ottawa, Ontario, Canada
The Work Extrinsic and Intrinsic Motivation Scale (WEIMS) is an 18-item measure of work motivation
theoretically grounded in self-determination theory (Deci & Ryan, 2000). The purpose of the present
research was twofold. First, the applicability of the WEIMS in different work environments was
evaluated. Second, its factorial structure and psychometric properties were assessed. Two samples of
workers (military: N ? 465; civilians: N ? 192) voluntarily completed questionnaires. Using the
WEIMS’s 3 indexes (work self-determination index, work self-determined and nonself-determined
motivation, respectively), results of regression analyses were supportive of its ability to predict positive
and negative criteria in the workplace. Results also showed the adequacy of both its construct validity and
internal consistency. Its factorial structure was also invariant across samples. Finally, its quasi-simplex
pattern and relationships with psychological correlates further supported the self-determination contin-
uum. Overall, these findings provide evidence for the applicability as well as the reliability and validity
of the WEIMS in organisational settings. Results are discussed in regard to the applicability of
self-determination theory to the workplace.
Keywords: work motivation, self-determination theory, scale validation
Work motivation is an enigmatic topic in work and organisa-
tional science (Kanfer, Chen, & Pritchard, 2008). Given today’s
economy, a motivated workforce represents both a competitive
advantage and a critical strategic asset in any work environment. In
organisational research, work motivation has been the subject of
more theories than any other topic (Baron, 1991); organisational
researchers see employee motivation as a fundamental building
block in the development of effective theories (Steers, Mowday, &
Shapiro, 2004). Indeed, programs of research guided by
expectancy-valance theory, self-regulation and goal-setting formu-
lations, social exchange and justice approaches, and self-
perspective (e.g., self-determination theory [SDT]; Deci & Ryan,
1985, 2000) have stimulated the development of organisational
and managerial practises to promote positive worker attitudes (e.g.,
employee commitment) and enhance job performance (e.g., indi-
vidual and team effort).
An issue that warrants attention in motivation research is the
method and approach used to assess this construct. Assessments of
employee motivation need to be practical, fast, flexible, and ac-
cessible through different means. Short, theory-grounded measures
leading to concrete applied venues are key to addressing these
organisational needs. This paper will thusly define and review
different approaches to studying and assessing motivation in the
workplace. Emphasis will be given to a subjective approach
grounded in SDT, which should prove valuable and practical for
use in rapidly changing organisational environments.
Definition and Types of Motivation Measure
Pinder (1998) defined work motivation as “a set of energetic
forces that originates both within as well as beyond an individual’s
being, to initiate work-related behaviour, and to determine its
form, direction, intensity and duration” (p. 11). Motivation is
thusly manifested by attention, effort, and persistence. The ability
to measure factors that energize, channel, and sustain work behav-
iour over time (Steers et al., 2004), is essential for capturing
employee motivation and for developing interventions aimed at
enhancing motivation, and in turn, job satisfaction and perfor-
mance. To date, most research on the influence of individual
factors in work motivation has investigated differences that can be
captured through self-report measures of personality, affect, inter-
ests, and values (Kanfer et al., 2008). Within the organisational
psychology literature, there are four major measurement systems
used to assess work motivation. These include projective, objec-
tive, implicit/explicit, and subjective measures.
The hallmark of a projective assessment is presenting the indi-
vidual with an ambiguous stimulus and eliciting a fairly unstruc-
tured response. As they apply to motivation, the vast majority are
designed to measure motivational needs, motives, or personality
traits (e.g., Thematic Apperception Test; Murray, 1943), but rarely
states or processes. Although there is arguably some support for
their criterion-related validity (e.g., Miner, 2002), their construct
validity is dependent on such boundary conditions as ensuring that
the criteria correspond to the underlying theory (e.g., Ployhart,
Schneider, & Schmitt, 2006). Their use in organisational settings
Maxime A. Tremblay, Ce´line M. Blanchard, Sara Taylor, and Luc G.
Pelletier, School of Psychology, University of Ottawa; Martin Villeneuve,
Department of National Defence, Ottawa, Ontario, Canada.
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Maxime
A. Tremblay, School of Psychology, University of Ottawa, 145 Jean-
Jacques Lussier, Montpetit 416 A, Ottawa, Ontario, Canada, K1N 6N5.
E-mail: [email protected]
Canadian Journal of Behavioural Science © 2009 Canadian Psychological Association
2009, Vol. 41, No. 4, 213–226 0008-400X/09/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/a0015167
213
has also diminished in the past few decades because they are not
necessarily specific to the domain of work (Ployhart, 2008).
Because objective measures minimise human judgement rel-
ative to projective measures, many researchers prefer them
(e.g., psychomotor measures such as pursuit rotor and finger
tapping; see Kanfer & Heggestad, 1997). However, although
their reliability is frequently high, a meta-analysis suggests this
is not always the case (Roth, Huffcutt, & Bobko, 2003). There
can be problems due to low baseline rates, strong influences by
environmental factors, and criterion deficiency (Ployhart,
2008). The assessment of behavioural indicators of work mo-
tivation also makes it challenging to delineate what is unique to
motivation and what belongs to the consequences (e.g., organi-
sational identification and job satisfaction; Weege, Van Dick,
Fisher, Wecking, & Moltzen, 2006). Although objective assess-
ments can be a useful way to measure choice, effort, and
persistence, simply using a measure because it is objective does
not make it construct valid, or more valid than any other
alternative (Ployhart, 2008).
From a theoretical perspective, there is growing convergence
amongst motivation scholars for the use of implicit (subcon-
scious) processes (e.g., James & Mazerolle, 2002; Kehr, 2004;
Locke & Latham, 2004). A key feature of implicit measures
(e.g., Implicit Attitude Test; Greenwald & Banaji, 1995) is that
the individual tends not to know (or be aware of) what is being
measured, therefore minimising socially desirable responses.
Implicit measures are thusly ideally suited for assessing socially
unpopular, sensitive, or controversial topics as well as uncon-
scious goals (see Johnson & Steinman, 2009). A drawback of
using implicit measures is that they do not allow researchers to
identify an individual’s true scores on latent constructs (Blanton
& Jaccard, 2006). In addition, the correlations between objec-
tive and subjective measures are often low, suggesting the
assessment of distinct phenomena (Thierry, 1990). The devel-
opment of a valid measure of conscious work motives, based on
theory, remains a critical step toward these new lines of re-
search.
Last, but not least, self-report measures are the most commonly
used measure of an employee’s motivation. Nevertheless, con-
tamination in subjective measures can also come from several
sources. For example, common-source bias occurs when the
same participants complete all of the measures in a study,
whereas common-method bias occurs when all measures are of
the same type (e.g., self-report) or assessed at the same time.
Arguably they can result in biased effect sizes (i.e., inflation or
attenuation), although some researchers do not believe the
results are always damaging (Ployhart, 2008).
Studies on individual differences in work motivation have been
investigated using peer ratings (Landy & Guion, 1970). Others
have documented individual differences within the framework of
Deci and Ryan’s (1985; Ryan & Deci, 2002) SDT. This includes
satisfaction with a given task and intentions to continue (as indi-
cated on questionnaires; Thierry, 1990). In the early 1990s, Blais,
Lachance, Vallerand, Brie`re, and Riddle (1993) were amongst the
first to provide empirical support for a SDT-based self-assessment
of work motivation. They developed a French instrument,
“L’Inventaire des Motivations au Travail de Blais” (Blais Inven-
tory of Work Motivation; BIWM). To this date, no one has
developed and validated an English version of this inventory.
Self-Determination Theory
SDT focuses on the “nature” of motivation, that is, the “why of
behaviour.” The underlying assumption is that “human beings are
active, growth-oriented organisms who are naturally inclined to-
ward integration of their psychic elements into a unified sense of
self and integration of themselves into larger social structures”
(Deci & Ryan, 2000, p. 229). Although psychological growth and
integration tendencies are natural, they are susceptible to social
and environmental conditions which can either support propensi-
ties for self-determination or disrupt them (Ryan & Deci, 2002).
SDT generally applies to activities that people find interesting,
optimally challenging, or aesthetically pleasing. Activities, which
are not experienced as such, work for example, are unlikely to be
performed unless there is, to some extent, an extrinsic reason for
doing them (Deci & Ryan, 2002). Consequently, SDT distin-
guishes between intrinsic motivation (i.e., doing an activity for its
own sake because one finds the activity inherently interesting and
satisfying) and extrinsic motivation (i.e., doing an activity for an
instrumental reason). There are different types of extrinsic moti-
vation that can be relatively controlled by external factors or that
can be relatively autonomous (i.e., self-regulated through an indi-
vidual’s acquired goals and values). These types of motivation can
be aligned along a continuum, that is, a quasi-simplex pattern
(Ryan & Connell, 1989) representing the degree to which goals/
values have been internalised (Ryan & Deci, 2002).
At the low-end lies amotivation (AMO) in which individuals
either lack the intention to act or act passively. Next along the
continuum is external regulation (ER), namely, doing an activity
only to obtain a reward. Next is introjected regulation (INTRO),
namely the regulation of behaviour through self-worth contingen-
cies (e.g., self-esteem, guilt). Then there is identified regulation
(IDEN), which refers to doing an activity because one identifies
with its value or meaning, and accepts it as one’s own. Finally,
there is integrated regulation (INTEG), which refers to identifying
with the value of an activity to the point that it becomes part of the
individual’s sense of self. This is the form of extrinsic motivation
that is most fully internalised and hence is said to be autonomous.
Identification, integration, and intrinsic motivation are the
prototype of self-determined motivations whereas amotivation,
external regulation, and introjection are categorized as nonself-
determined motivations. SDT does not presuppose that the self-
determination continuum is a developmental one in the sense that
individuals progress along it in specific stages. Rather, a new
behaviour may be internalised at any point along the continuum
depending on factors such as organisational context and an indi-
vidual’s prior experiences (Ryan, 1995).
Motivational Correlates
The self-determination continuum is useful for predicting
“optimal functioning.” Optimal functioning in organisations in-
cludes employee engagement, job performance subjective well-
being, and retention (Gagne´ & Forest, 2008). SDT states that
intrinsic motivation leads to the most positive consequences, fol-
lowed by integrated and identified regulations. Introjected and
external regulations lead to negative outcomes. Amotivation re-
sults in the most negative consequences (Vallerand & Ratelle,
2002). These negative outcomes may include counterproductive
performance and employee withdrawal.
214
TREMBLAY ET AL.
Most studies on SDT have been validated in nonorganisational
settings (e.g., Academic Motivation Scale: Vallerand, Blais,
Brie`re, & Pelletier, 1989; Leisure Motivation Scale: Pelletier,
Vallerand, Blais, Brie`re, & Green-Demers, 1996) Blais and his
colleagues (1993) were the first to provide empirical support for
the effect of the self-determination continuum in a work setting,
using their French 31-item BIWM. Their results indicated that
external and introjected regulations were associated with emo-
tional exhaustion, and physical and mental health problems (see
also Houkes, Jassen, de Jonge, & Bakker, 2003). Self-determined
types of motivation were also shown to display positive associa-
tions with job and life satisfaction (see also Locke & Latham,
2004).
Gagne´ and Deci (2005) argued that a supportive work climate
satisfied the fundamental psychological needs of autonomy, com-
petence, and relatedness postulated by SDT (Deci & Ryan, 1985).
Such climate affects employees’ intrinsic motivation and, in turn,
increases organisational citizenship behaviours. Autonomy-
supportive interpersonal styles have been shown to enhance intrin-
sic motivation and, in turn, such positive work-related outcomes as
subordinates’ perceptions, affects, and satisfactions (e.g., Deci,
Connell, & Ryan, 1989; Zuckerman, Porac, Lathin, Smith, &
Deci, 1978). In their respective integrative models of work
motivation, both Locke and Latham (2004) and Meyer, Becker,
and Vandenberghe (2004) proposed theoretical associations be-
tween work motivation, job involvement, and organisational
commitment. Better job performance, employee engagement,
subjective well-being, and employee retention also have been
studied as indicators of optimal functioning in the workplace
(e.g., Baard, Deci, & Ryan, 2004; Meyer & Gagne´, 2008).
On the other hand, negative outcomes that have been researched
in terms of their associations with low work motivation include
depression (e.g., Blais et al., 1993) and turnover intentions (e.g.,
Quast & Kleinbeck, 1990). For instance, specific relationships
between low intrinsic motivation, job burnout, and voluntary turn-
over intentions have been found in samples of bank employees and
teachers (Houkes, Janssen, de Jonge, & Nijhuis, 2001). Although
there is relatively little previous work relating SDT concepts to
workplace deviance, (contrasting) evidence does indicate that au-
tonomous motivation promotes volunteering and other prosocial
behaviours (e.g., Gagne´, 2003) and thusly, presumably would
predict lower workplace defiant behaviours. These results are quite
promising. Thusly it would appear important to evaluate an En-
glish version of the BIWM using a self-report approach to mea-
suring work motivation. Doing so would allow the English re-
search community to assess the multidimensional aspects of
motivation. To date, very few theory-driven self-report measures
of employee motivation are available for researchers and practi-
tioners. The measures that do exist are often limited to intrinsic
motivation (Hackman & Oldham, 1975; Warr, Cook, & Wall,
1979). Although the French version of the BIWM has demon-
strated adequate psychometric properties (Blais et al., 1993;
Vallerand, 1997), the constructs it measures are tapped by 31
items. This increases both scale complexity and participants’ com-
pletion time. Moreover, Ryan and Deci‘s (2002) concept of inte-
gration (i.e., when identification by an individual has been evalu-
ated and brought into congruence with the person’s values, goals,
and needs) was introduced after Blais et al.’s validation of their
instrument and, thusly, cannot be measured with the current
BIWM. It is in part for these reasons that inspired by Blais’s
original work, a new organisational research tool assessing work
motivation was developed.
Overview of Studies
The purpose of this research was to test the applicability (and
versatility) of the WEIMS in different work environments as well
as to evaluate its factorial structure and psychometric properties.
Three studies were conducted,
• Study 1 assessed the six-factor, three-indicator factorial struc-
ture of the WEIMS using confirmatory factor analysis. Its internal
consistency and construct validity were also examined.
• Study 2 assessed psychological constructs (both antecedents
and consequences) hypothesised to be related to work motivation;
and
• Study 3, together with Study 2, assessed the criterion validity
of the WEIMS for predicting positive and negative work-related
consequences using three indexes: the self-determination index
and the two forms of motivation (i.e., self-determined and nonself-
determined motivation). Its factorial invariance was also exam-
ined.
Preliminary Validation Steps
To develop the 18-item WEIMS, the best three manifest mea-
surement indicators (items) of each of the five BIWM’s original
constructs,
1
were adapted using a back-to-back retranslation tech-
nique (Vallerand, 1989). Following this adaptation step, the
WEIMS’s 18 items were put to the test of an exploratory factor
analysis (EFA) using a sample of Anglophone health care workers
(100 women and 9 men; mean age ? 44). The results of this EFA
(maximum likelihood extraction with oblique rotation) were con-
clusive. The six-factor structure of the WEIMS was supported,
with three items (per latent construct) serving as indicators. All 18
items had loadings higher than .30 (Tabachnick & Fidell, 2001).
Moreover, every item loaded on their respective latent construct
(eigenvalues greater then 1): intrinsic motivation (IM4 ? .41;
IM8 ? .34; IM15 ? .40), integrated regulation (INTEG5 ? .44;
INTEG10 ? .41; INTEG18 ? .42), identified regulation
(IDEN1 ? .45; IDEN7 ? .63; IDEN14 ? .33), introjected regu-
lation (INTRO6 ? .44; INTRO11 ? .38; INTRO13 ? .56),
external regulation (EXT2 ? .87; EXT9 ? .82; EXT16 ? .70),
and amotivation (AMO3 ? .36; AMO12 ? .44; AMO17 ? .34).
Based on these preliminary results, the WEIMS is a shorter version
of the French questionnaire and yet assesses all six motivational
constructs postulated by SDT.
1
Examples are as follows: IM4 « Parce que j’ai beaucoup de plaisir a`
apprendre de nouvelles choses dans ce travail »; IDEN7 « Parce que c’est
le type de travail que j’ai choisi pour re´aliser mes projets de carrie`re »;
INTRO11 « Parce que je tiens absolument a` eˆtre tre`s bon(ne) dans ce
travail, sinon je serais de´c¸u(e) »; EXT9 « Parce que cela me permet de faire
de l’argent »; AMO12 « Je ne le sais pas, on nous fixe des conditions
de travail irre´alistes ». The integration items were generated based on the
Global Motivation Scale (Guay, Blais, Vallerand, & Pelletier, 1999). An
example of item adaptation from global to work is: INTEG5 “Because they
reflect what I value most in life” into “Because it has become a funda-
mental part of who I am.”
215
SPECIAL SECTION: WORK MOTIVATION: WEIMS
Study 1 and Study 2
As noted above, the objectives of Study 1 were threefold: (a) the
factorial structure of the WEIMS was examined through confir-
matory factor analysis, (b) the internal consistencies of the six
motivational subscales were assessed, and (c) the construct validity
of the WEIMS was examined by conducting item-to-total inter-
correlations as well as correlations amongst the subscales. It was
hypothesised that each of the 18 indicators of the WEIMS would
respectively load on six separate latent constructs (i.e., three items
per factor). Moreover, the scale was expected to show adequate to
excellent item-to-item consistency, along with midrange-to-high
item-to-total correlations. Finally, based on Ryan and Connell’s
(1989) research, the WEIMS’s quasi-simplex pattern of relation-
ships was expected to display a self-determination continuum in
which adjacent subscales show strong positive correlations (e.g.,
INTEG and IDEN), whereas subscales at opposite ends of the
continuum display the most negative correlations (i.e., IM and
AMO).
Study 2 was designed to present the first of two ways of using
the WEIMS for prediction purposes, namely the work self-
determination index (W–SDI). It was hypothesised that perceived
organisational support and work climate would be predicted by the
W–SDI. It was also anticipated that higher W–SDI would posi-
tively relate to job satisfaction and organisational commitment,
and negatively associate with work strain and turnover intentions.
Finally, it was hypothesised that lower W–SDI would show the
reverse pattern of relationships.
A final objective of Study 2 was to examine the associations
between each type of motivation and work-related antecedents and
consequences. As evidence of criterion validity, it was hypoth-
esised that work self-determined types of motivation (i.e., IM,
INTEG, IDEN) would positively correlate with the antecedents
(i.e., organisational support and work climate), job satisfaction and
organisational commitment, as well as negatively correlate with
work strain and turnover intentions; that the first two types of work
nonself-determined motivation (i.e., INTRO, EXT) would show
relatively low correlations with these same variables and that
amotivation would demonstrate the reverse pattern of relation-
ships.
Method
Participants and Procedure
A sample of 600 Regular Force military members across Canada
received the Omnibus Survey of the Canadian Forces (CF). The
survey sample was derived by a random stratified sampling pro-
cedure intended to accurately reflect particular demographic char-
acteristics, regarding gender, rank, and language. Participants were
invited to complete the survey and mail it to the CF’s Director of
Human Resources. Return envelopes and postage were provided.
Participation in the survey was voluntary, and the anonymity and
confidentiality of responses were assured. A total of 465 CF members
returned the completed surveys for a response rate of 77.5%.
For validation purposes, the CF sample was divided into two
groups, using the SPSS’ (version 11.5) random selection of cases
option. One group was assigned to either the construct validation
phase of the WEIMS (Study 1), or to its content validity assess-
ment (Study 2). Unique data files were thusly used for different
phases of validation. By doing so, sample-specific bias that may
result in performing both confirmatory factor analysis (CFA) and
correlations with dependent participants was minimised. Conse-
quently, the sample for Study 1 (N ? 205) consisted of 166 men
and 21 women (18 individuals did not report their gender) from
which 90.6% were English-speaking Regular Force members
(72.9%) who had been serving in the CF for an average of 18.16
years. Study 2 (N ? 260) was also comprised of Regular Force
members (77.3%) of which 223 were men and 21 were women (16
individuals omitted to indicate their gender; 89.6% English
2
; 18.20
years of CF service).
Measures
Work Motivation
Participants completed the 18-item WEIMS (see Appendix A).
The WEIMS is divided into three-item six subscales, which cor-
respond to the six types of motivation postulated by SDT (i.e.,
intrinsic motivation, integrated, identified, introjected and external
regulations, and amotivation). Participants were asked to indicate
on a Likert-type scale
3
ranging from 1 (does not correspond at all)
to 5 (corresponds exactly) the extent to which the items represent
the reasons they are presently involved in their work.
Although most researchers favour a multidimensional approach
to (work) motivation, the use of a single score, such as the work
self-determination index (W–SDI; Vallerand, 1997), is at times be
desirable. The W–SDI may be particularly useful when researchers
want to select individuals who display either a self-determined or
a nonself-determined motivational profile. The WEIMS can be
used to generate that index by multiplying the mean of each
subscale by weights corresponding to the underlying level of
self-determination (Ryan & Connell, 1989). The formula for de-
termining the W–SDI is as follows: W–SDI ? (?3 ? IM) ?
(?2 ? INTEG) ? (?1 ? IDEN) ? (?1 ? INTRO) ? (?2 ?
EXT) ? (?3 ? AMO). The range of possible scores on the
W–SDI is between ? 36 for a 7-point Likert-type scale (and ? 24
when using a 5-point Likert-type scale). The total score derived
from this formula reflects individuals’ relative level of self-
determination. A positive score indicates a self-determined profile
and a negative score indicates a nonself-determined profile. Pre-
vious research has shown that the self-determination index dis-
plays high levels of reliability and validity (e.g., Fortier, Vallerand,
& Guay, 1995; Green-Demers, Pelletier, & Me´nard, 1997;
Pelletier, Dion, Slovinec-D’Angelo, & Reid, 2004). The internal
consistency (Cronbach’s alpha coefficient) of the W–SDI was .84.
Perceived Organisational Support
The short version of the Survey of Perceived Organisational
Support (Eisenberg, Huntington, Hutchison, & Sowa, 1986) was
2
The approximate 10% of remaining participants on both samples who
did not indicate English as their first language were for the majority
French. To get a job offer in most departments of the federal government,
a bilingual fluency is often required.
3
For conceptual purposes, the Omnibus Survey of the CF had a frame-
work consisting of only 5-point Likert-type scales (or less). For this reason,
the original 7-point scale was reduced to a 5-point scale for this sample.
216
TREMBLAY ET AL.
used to measure CF members’ perceptions of being appreciated
and cared for by their organisation. It consists of 16 Likert-type
items for which responses can range from 1 (do not agree at all)
to 5 (agree completely). The internal consistency for this motiva-
tional antecedent was .90.
Work Climate
Organisational climate was defined as members’ perceptions of
how they are being treated by their organisation (Villeneuve &
Gingras, 1998). The questions measuring work climate addressed
six dimensions: (a) involvement, (b) consideration, (c) efficacy
and fairness of the rules, (d) quality of feedback, (e) autonomy, and
(f) recognition/encouragement. The participants were asked to
describe the CF by answering 26 items rated across a Likert-type
scale ranging from 1 (do not agree at all) to 5 (agree completely).
The alpha value for these combined subscales was .97.
Organisational Commitment
Affective commitment was defined as emotional attachment to
the organisation, and continuance commitment as recognition of
the costs associated with leaving the organisation. Each subscale of
this Affective and Continuance Commitment Scale (Allen &
Meyer, 1990) consists of eight Likert-type items with response
category ranging from 1 (do not agree at all) to 5 (agree com-
pletely). The internal consistency for the combined affective and
continuance components was .66.
Job Satisfaction
The Job Satisfaction Scale (Hackman & Oldham, 1975) is an
instrument consisting of 31 items which can be rated along a
Likert-type scale ranging from 1 (do not agree at all) to 5 (agree
completely). The dimensions assessed include satisfaction with: (a)
nature of the job; (b) salary and benefits; (c) promotion potential
and recognition; (d) working conditions; (e) supervision, peers,
and the organisation; (f) job security; (g) geographical location of
the workplace; and (h) comparative value of the job. The internal
consistency was .90 for the combination of these subscales.
Work Strain
Individual ill-being (or strain) was assessed using the 20-item
Symptoms Checklist (Bartone, Ursano, Wright, & Ingraham, 1989).
This checklist assessed the frequency, using a Likert-type scale rang-
ing from 0 (never) to 3 (very often), with which symptoms were
experienced in four domains: (a) depression/withdrawal, (b) hypera-
lertness, (c) generalised anxiety, and (d) and somatic complaints. The
internal consistency of the total index was .95.
Turnover Intentions
The intent to stay (or leave) an organisation is a predictor of
active human behaviour. CF members were asked, using the Re-
tention and Attrition Questionnaire (adapted from Bernard, 2001),
to describe their current career aspirations on a Likert-type scale
ranging from 1 (do not agree at all) to 5 (agree completely). The
dimensions addressed included: (a) intent to leave the CF/
Department of National Defence, (b) intent to pursue a posting out
of the Unit, and (c) intent to stay in the Unit but change job. The
internal consistency was .63.
Results
Study 1: Construct Validation
Confirmatory Factor Analysis
CFA was used in Study 1 because it is a powerful approach for
evaluating measurement models by allowing researchers to test the
hypothesised factor structure of a set of items (Ployhart, 2008).
CFA also provides a more stringent test of the underlying factor
structure of a survey instrument than any other method, including
EFA (Bollen, 1989; Jo¨reskog & So¨rbom, 1989) and multitrait/
multimethod approaches (Bagozzi, Yi, & Phillips, 1991). CFA
also provides goodness-of-fit indexes for the resulting solution
(Anderson & Gerbing, 1988; Kenny & Kasby, 1992). Conse-
quently, CFA was used to determine whether the factorial structure
of the WEIMS for this military sample resembled the original
five-factor model of work motivation validated by Blais and his
colleagues (1993) using a Francophone sample. The integrated
regulation construct was also included in the present study. CFA
was conducted including all 18 items, with three indicators mea-
suring each type of motivation and was performed on the covari-
ance matrix generated by EQS 6.1 (Bentler, 2005) using the
maximum likelihood (ML) estimation method for testing the full
six-factor model. Table 1 presents descriptive statistics for each of
the 18 work motivation indicators as well as individual item-to-
total (for each subscale) correlations.
CFA rendered a satisfactory fit: ?
2
(120, N ? 203) ? 185.562,
p ? .001 (?
2
/df ratio ? 1.55; Kline, 1998); Comparative Fit Index
(CFI) ? .958, standardized root mean square residual (SRMR) ?
.062, root mean square error of approximation (RMSEA) ? .046,
90% confidence interval (CI) RMSEA ? .028, .062. Also, all
items had standardised factor loadings over .30 (ranging from
.30 to 93). Each set of three indicators also showed midrange-
to-high item-to-total correlations (all above .50), representing a
first indication of construct validity. The final model is depicted
in Figure 1.
Internal Consistency
In line with the descriptive statistics presented in Table 2, the
results indicated that external regulation (M ? 3.75) was the main
reason why CF members were involved in their work. As well,
along the diagonal of Table 2 are presented the Cronbach’s alphas
assessing the internal consistency of the WEIMS’s six subscales.
Alpha values ranged from .64 to .83, suggesting adequate reliabil-
ity (IM?.80; INTEG ?.83; IDEN ?.67; INTRO ?.70; EXT ?
.77; AMO ? .64). Overall, considering the fact that these sub-
scales consist of only three indicators each, they show adequate
internal consistency. As well, their respective alpha values are
approximately the same as those obtained with the original French
scale.
4
4
The alpha values of the original French scale were as follow: IM?.77;
IDEN ? .75; INTRO ? .74; EXT ? .73; AMO ? .59 (see Blais et al.,
1993, for complete details).
217
SPECIAL SECTION: WORK MOTIVATION: WEIMS
Content Validity
Pearson correlations computed amongst the six subscales are
also presented in Table 2. They were expected to display the
presence of a self-determination continuum (Ryan & Connell,
1989). Overall, the quasi-simplex pattern of the present sample is
in agreement with the one obtained with the French version of the
WEIMS (see Blais et al., 1993). This too provides some prelimi-
nary support for the construct validity of this English version.
More interesting, introjection yielded a relatively high relationship
with integration (r ? .63, p ? .01). As well, external regulation
was not significantly related to intrinsic motivation (as was the
case in the French validation) and the absence of motivation only
yielded significance with integration (r ? ?.20, p ? .01).
Study 2: Work Self-Determination Index and
Criterion Validity
As an effort to ensure normal distribution patterns for all mea-
sured variables, descriptive statistics were first examined and are
presented in Table 3 (i.e., work motivation) and Table 4 (i.e.,
work-related antecedents and consequences). Overall, variables
displayed normal distributions. In line with the findings of Study
1, results indicated, here again, external regulation (M ? 3.63) to
be the main underlying motive of CF’s membership. For this
sample, the mean value for the W–SDI was equal to ? 2.96,
indicating a rather low self-determined profile. As well, Cron-
bach’s alphas, further assessing the internal consistency of the six
subscales, are presented in Table 3 and were also suggesting fairly
good reliability (alphas ranging from .60 to .84).
Work Self-Determination Index
Regression analyses were conducted to examine the relationship
between the WEIMS and work-related antecedents and conse-
quences. Results of the linear regression analyses for the W–SDI
are presented in Table 4.
As hypothesised, both organisational support (? ? .27, p ?
.001) and work climate (? ? .26, p ? .001) were significantly
linked to the W–SDI (R
2
? .23). As well, the W–SDI was
positively linked to job satisfaction (? ? .57, p ? .001, R
2
? .33)
and organisational commitment (? ? .33, p ? .001, R
2
? .11). It
was also negatively linked to work strain (? ? ?.29, p ? .001,
R
2
?.08) and turnover intentions (? ??.48, p ?.001, R
2
?.23).
In sum, these findings indicate that a positive work environment is
accompanied by higher levels of work self-determination, and that
.56
.80
.84
.80 .79 .76 .09 -.23
.81
.74
.79 .89 .83 .11 -.34
.39
.74
. .78
.62 .73 .04 -.33
.76
.67
.92 .14 -.24
.68
.61
.01
.34
.85
.65
IM4
IM8
IM15
INTEG5
INTEG10
INTEG18
IDEN1
IDEN7
IDEN14
INTRO6
INTRO11
INTRO13
EXT2
EXT9
EXT16
AMO3
AMO12
AMO17
IM
INTEG
IDEN
INTRO
AMO
EXT
.83
.61
.54
.79
.65
.74
.39
.73
.80
.58
.67
.61
.92
.67
.63
.94
.52
.76
Figure 1. Confirmatory factor analysis of the Work Extrinsic and Intrin-
sic Motivation Scale (Study 1). IM ? intrinsic motivation; INTEG ?
integrated regulation; IDEN ? identified regulation; EXT ? external
regulation; INTRO ? introjected regulation; AMO ? amotivation.
Table 1
Means, Standard Deviations, Skewness, and Item-to-Total
Correlations for Individual Items of the WEIMS (Study 1)
Descriptive statistics
Variable M SD Skew r
Intrinsic motivation
IM4 3.61 1.08 ?.415 .79
IM8 3.59 1.50 ?.467 .88
IM15 3.57 1.59 ?.476 .86
Integrated regulation
INTEG5 3.06 1.30 ?.203 .85
INTEG10 3.47 1.20 ?.259 .88
INTEG18 3.22 1.21 ?.123 .86
Identified regulation
IDEN1 2.91 1.27 ?.008 .73
IDEN7 3.05 1.28 ?.167 .80
IDEN14 2.98 1.08 ?.071 .80
Introjected regulation
INTRO6 3.43 1.29 ?0.482 .79
INTRO11 3.41 1.22 ?0.466 .84
INTRO13 3.20 1.34 ?0.163 .76
External regulation
EXT2 3.68 1.13 ?0.519 .84
EXT9 3.80 1.24 ?0.716 .76
EXT16 3.76 1.14 ?0.482 .88
Amotivation
AMO3 1.52 .80 1.388 .59
AMO12 2.50 1.28 0.504 .86
AMO17 2.18 1.08 4.060 .81
Note. N ? 205. Items were rated on a 5-point Likert-type scale ranging
from 1 (does not correspond at all) to 5 (corresponds exactly). rs were
significant at p ? .01. WEIMS ? Work Extrinsic and Intrinsic Motivation
Scale; IM ? intrinsic motivation; INTRO ? introjected regulation; IN-
TEG ? integrated regulation; EXT ? external regulation; IDEN ? iden-
tified regulation; AMO ? amotivation.
218
TREMBLAY ET AL.
the higher the value of one’s work self-determination profile, the
more engaged in and satisfied with his or her job the individual is
likely to be. As well, lower should his or her chances of experi-
encing work strain and willingness to leave the organisation.
Criterion Validity
Correlations between the WEIMS’s subscales and a series of
psychological constructs that were considered either motivational
antecedents or consequences also appear in Table 4. As hypoth-
esised, perceived organisational support and positive work climate
correlated positively with four out of the five types of motivation
(rs ranging from .24 to .41, p ? .01), and correlated negatively
with amotivation (r ? ?.23 and ?.25, p ? .01, respectively).
These two antecedents did not correlate significantly with external
regulation.
Job satisfaction and organisational commitment correlated
positively with work self-determined types of motivation (rs
ranging between .40 and .46 for satisfaction, p ? .01, and .32
to .41 for commitment) and yielded weaker positive correla-
tions with introjected regulation (rs between .32 to .34, p ?
.01). Whereas external regulation was only positively correlated
to commitment (r ? .13, p ? .05), amotivation yielded negative
correlations with both constructs (r ? ?.13 and ?.34, ps ?
.05). Also, work strain was positively correlated to amotivation
(r ? .33, p ? .01), yielding no significant correlations with the
other five variables. Finally, with the exception of external
regulation, significant negative correlations were found be-
tween turnover intentions and all of the types of work motiva-
tion (i.e., self-determined: rs between ?.35 to ?.47, ps ? .01;
nonself-determined: rs between ?.20 and ?.26, p ? .01).
These results further demonstrate how each form of motivation
relates differently to work-related criteria.
Study 3
Study 3 further examined the criterion related validity of the
WEIMS for predicting various organisational criteria. This was
done by presenting the second of two ways of using the WEIMS
for prediction purposes, namely the use of the two forms of
work motivation (i.e., work self-determined motivation (W–
SDM) versus nonself-determined motivation (W–NSDM)).
Whereas W–SDM was hypothesised to be linked to positive
consequences, mainly organisational involvement, commitment
and citizenship behaviours, W–NSDM was hypothesised to be
less so linked to those variables as well as to be positively
Table 2
Means, Standard Deviations, Internal Consistency Values (Cronbach’s alpha), and Pearson
Correlations for the WEIMS’s Subscales (Study 1)
Statistics WEIMS’s subscales
Variable M SD IM INTEG IDEN INTRO EXT AMO
INTEG 3.29 1.07 (.83) .67 .63 .19 ?.20
IDEN 2.98 0.94 (.67) .49 .16 ?.12
INTRO 3.35 1.02 (.70) .16 ?.13
EXT 3.75 0.99 (.77) .03
AMO 2.07 0.82 (.64)
Note. N ? 205. All correlations above .16 are significant at p ? .01. WEIMS ? Work Extrinsic and Intrinsic
Motivation Scale; IM ? intrinsic motivation; INTEG ? integrated regulation; IDEN ? identified regulation;
INTRO ? introjected regulation; EXT ? external regulation; AMO ? amotivation.
Table 3
Means, Standard Deviations, Variance, Skewness, and Alpha Values for the WEIMS’s Subscales
and the W–SDI (Study 2)
Descriptive statistics
Variable M SD Variance Skewness ?
WEIMS subscales
IM 3.49 0.96 .91 ?.48 .77
INTEG 3.18 1.11 1.23 ?.32 .84
IDEN 2.92 1.01 1.01 .02 .74
INTRO 3.12 1.07 1.14 ?.18 .71
EXT 3.63 1.07 1.13 ?.53 .81
AMO 2.14 0.85 0.73 .53 .60
W–SDI 2.96 5.39 29.03 ?.09 .84
Note. N ? 260. Items were rated on a 5-point Likert-type scale ranging from 1 (does not correspond at all)
to 5 (corresponds exactly). WEIMS ? Work Extrinsic and Intrinsic Motivation Scale; W–SDI ? Work
Self-Determination Index; IM ? intrinsic motivation; INTEG ? integrated regulation; IDEN ? identified
regulation; INTRO ? introjected regulation; EXT ? external regulation; AMO ? amotivation.
219
SPECIAL SECTION: WORK MOTIVATION: WEIMS
linked to more negative ones that is workplace deviant behav-
iours. In terms of scale validation, Study 3 also evaluated the
factorial invariance of the WEIMS across samples (i.e., Study 1
and 2: military vs. Study 3: civilian). It was hypothesised that
the WEIMS’s set of 18 indicators would assess the same six
latent constructs (i.e., types of motivation) in both samples.
Method
Participants and Procedure
A sample of 192 workers from diverse organisations in the
Ottawa–Gatineau region completed a questionnaire during their
free time and returned it to the researchers by mail. Return
envelopes and postage were provided. Participation in the study
was voluntary, and participants were informed that their re-
sponses would be anonymous and kept confidential. A total of
112 women and 76 men completed the questionnaire (4 indi-
viduals did not indicate their gender). The mean age of partic-
ipants was 35.79. The majority of the sample was federal
government employees (45.3%; national organisation: 33.3%;
multinational organisation: 28.6%), whereas the others worked
either in the service industry (28.2%) and/or sale/retail (19.3%;
small office: 34.4%). Participants earned an average salary
between $40,001 and $60,000 per annum (university degree:
46.5%; collegial certificate: 28.1%; high school diploma:
22.5%).
Measures
Work Motivation (See Study 1 for Complete Details)
Participants were asked to indicate their level of agreement with
each of the 18 items, using a Likert-type scale ranging from 1
(does not correspond at all) to 7 (corresponds exactly). In addition
to the W–SDI (described in the Method section of Study 2), the
WEIMS was used to examine the influence of work self-
determined as opposed to nonself-determined motivation. A score
for W–SDM can be generated by summing the means of each of
the three self-determined subscales (i.e., IM, INTEG, and IDEN).
Similarly, a score for W–NSDM can be obtained by summing the
means of the three nonself-determined subscales (i.e., INTRO,
EXT, and AMO). Either for prediction purposes or when testing
comprehensive theoretical models with techniques such as struc-
tural equation modelling, these two forms of work motivation are
useful when researchers need to reduce the number of indicators
(i.e., latent variables) to provide adequate tests of models. Internal
consistency values of .87 and .72 were obtained for work self-
determined and nonself-determined motivation, respectively.
Organisational Involvement
Involvement was measured through the use of a 12-item scale,
developed by Tyler and Blader (2002). Participants indicated their
agreement on a Likert-type scale ranging from 1 (strongly dis-
agree) to 6 (strongly agree). Internal consistency tests revealed an
alpha value of .85.
Organisational Commitment
Participants completed an aggregated 18-item measure of commit-
ment by indicating their agreement with each item on a Likert-type
scale ranging from 1 (never) to 7 (always). Five of the 18 items
measured general organisational commitment (Meyer &Allen, 1991),
7 tapped team-oriented commitment, and 6 measured career-oriented
commitment (Ellemers, de Gilder, & van den Heuvel, 1998). The
alpha coefficient for the combined subscales was .90.
Organisational Citizenship Behaviours
Organisational citizenship behaviours were assessed using a
13-item measure of working with others (Podsakoff, Ahearne,
Table 4
Correlations Between WEIMS Subscales and Motivational Antecedents and Consequences as
Well as ? and Adjusted R
2
From W–SDI Regression Analyses (Study 2)
Antecedents Consequences
Variable Support Climate Job Sat. Commit. Strain Turnover
M 2.60 2.74 2.84 2.93 21.58 2.58
SD 0.73 0.93 0.65 0.61 16.32 0.98
? .90 .97 .89 .67 .95 .65
WEIMS subscales
IM .34 .41 .46 .41 ?.06 ?.47
INTEG .34 .37 .45 .37 ?.12 ?.36
IDEN .26 .34 .40 .32 ?.08 ?.35
INTRO .24 .34 .34 .32 ?.01 ?.26
EXT ?.04 .07 .02 .13 .10 ?.03
AMO ?.23 ?.25 ?.34 ?.13 .33 .20
Work Self-Determination Index
W–SDI .27 .26 .57 .33 ?.29 ?.48
R
2
.23 .33 .11 .08 .23
Note. N ? 260. All correlations above .13 are significant at p ? .05. WEIMS ? Work Extrinsic and Intrinsic
Motivation Scale; IM ? intrinsic motivation; INTEG ? integrated regulation; IDEN ? identified regulation;
INTRO ? introjected regulation; EXT ? external regulation; AMO ? amotivation; W–SDI ? Work Self-
Determination Index; Job Sat. ? job satisfaction; Commit. ? commitment.
220
TREMBLAY ET AL.
& MacKenzie, 1997). Participants indicated their agreement on
a Likert-type scale ranging from 1 (never) to 7 (always). The
alpha value for this measure was .90.
Workplace Deviant Behaviours
Participants completed the Workplace Deviance Scale
(Bennett & Robinson, 2000) which is comprised of two sub-
scales: a 13-item measure of organisational deviance (deviant
behaviours directly harmful to the organisation), and a 7-item
measure of interpersonal deviance (deviant behaviours directly
harmful to other individuals within the organisation). Partici-
pants indicated the frequency to which each behaviour was
representative of their own on a Likert-type scale ranging from
1 (never) to 7 (always). An alpha value of .84 was obtained.
Results
Descriptive statistics were examined to assess normal distribu-
tion patterns for all measured variables. They are presented in
Table 5. Means of 4.35 and 3.73 were obtained for work self-
determined and nonself-determined motivation, respectively.
Overall, the variables followed normal distributions. In addition,
similar alpha values to the ones obtained for the CF sample, were
obtained for the subscales using this civilian sample (IM ? .87;
INTEG ? .80; IDEN ? .70; INTRO ? .76; EXT ? .73; AMO ?
.75).
To the exception of the absence of a correlation between work
nonself-determined motivation, organisational involvement and
commitment, hypotheses were generally supported. Moreover, the
two forms of motivation (i.e., W–SDM and W–NSDM) were
positively correlated (r ?.31, p ?.01). Significant correlations (rs
ranging from .20 to .66, ps ? .01) were also found between all
measured outcome variables (i.e., involvement, commitment, cit-
izenship, and deviance), to the exception of the link between
deviant behaviours and both organisational involvement and com-
mitment.
Work Self-Determined and Nonself-Determined
Motivation
Regression analyses were conducted to demonstrate the useful-
ness of the WEIMS in predicting positive as well as negative
organisational consequences, depending on one’s work self-
determined and nonself-determined motivation.
5
As expected,
W–SDM was positively linked to organisational involvement (? ?
.49, p ? .001), commitment (? ? .54, p ? .001), and citizenship
behaviours (? ?.44, p ?.001) as well negatively linked to deviant
behaviours (? ??.24, p ?.001); the individual more likely acting
(and reacting) positively toward the organisation and his or her
fellow coworkers. W–NSDM was found to be negatively linked to
citizenship behaviours (? ? ?.29, p ? .001) and positively linked
to deviant behaviours (? ? .25, p ? .001); the individual more
likely engaging in antisocial or organisational behaviours. No
significant relationships were found between W–NSDM, involve-
ment and commitment. Taken together, W–SDM and W–NSDM
explained 23% of the variance in organisational involvement, 27%
of work commitment, 19% of citizenship behaviours, and 7% of
workplace deviance. Results of the regression analyses for the two
forms of work motivation (i.e., W–SDM and W–NSDM) are
presented in Table 6.
Factorial Invariance
Although the WEIMS was identically specified for both military
and civilian samples, this does not guarantee the equivalence of
item measurements across the two groups (Kline, 1998). For
example, despite an identically specified factor loading, it is pos-
sible that with the imposition of equality constraints across groups,
the tenability of invariance would not hold. That is, the link
between a particular indicator and its target latent construct may be
sample specific (Byrne, 1994). Prior to the evaluation of the
WEIMS’s factorial invariance (i.e., military: Study 1 and 2, N ?
205, civilians: Study 3, N ? 192), scores from the 18 items were
first converted into z scores to render a more adequate invariance
assessment. This procedure was used because work motivation
was assessed by the WEIMS using two different Likert-type scales
(i.e., Study 1 and 2: 5 point; Study 3: 7 point).
Results for this test of the measurement model using structural
equation modelling yielded a satisfactory CFI value of .902,
?
2
(273, N ? 395) ? 540.315, p ? .001, ?
2
/df ratio ? 1.98,
SRMR ? .095, RMSEA ? .052, 90% CI RMSEA ? .045, .058.
This suggests that the hypothesised model invariance represents an
acceptable fit to the data. More important, an examination of
probability values associated with the LM univariate and multi-
variate chi-square test statistics revealed none of the WEIMS’s 18
items to be invariant across groups.
General Discussion
The purpose of the present research was to assess the applica-
bility of the WEIMS by testing its factorial structure and its
psychometric properties using different work environments. Three
studies were conducted.
A series of regression analyses revealed that the WEIMS has
construct, content, and criterion validity for organisational settings.
Replicating the correlational findings of previous studies, the
present results support the use of the work self-determination
index (Study 2) and are consistent with SDT’s assertion that as
self-determination rises, consequences become increasingly posi-
tive (e.g., job satisfaction and commitment), whereas lower levels
of self-determination result in more negative experiences (e.g.,
work strain, turnover intentions).
The present results also support the WEIMS’s ability to predict
positive and negative organisational criteria based on one’s work
self-determined motivation and work nonself-determined motiva-
tion (Study 3). As hypothesised, as self-determined motivation
5
The factor analytic support for the claim that the WEIMS can be
conceptualized as two forms of motivation, namely W–SDM and
W–NSDM was first tested by the mean of a second-order CFA (Byrne,
1994). In this second-order CFA, the six motivation types were hypothe-
sized to be explained by two second-order factors: W–SDM (comprised of
IM, INTEG, IDEN) and W–NSDM (comprised of INTRO, EXT, AMO).
Results yielded a CFI value of .919, ?
2
(130, N ? 192) ? 393.323, p ?
.001, ?
2
/df ratio ? 3.03, SRMR ? .081, RMSEA ? .116, 90% CI
RMSEA ?.072, .163; indicating that the hypothesized binary higher order
motivational structure represents an acceptable fit to the data.
221
SPECIAL SECTION: WORK MOTIVATION: WEIMS
increases, individuals are more likely to report being involved in
and committed to their work as well as connected and loyal to the
organisation. In contrast, higher levels of work nonself-
determinated motivation resulted in employees being less inclined
to help coworkers and being more likely to engage in deviant
behaviours.
More interesting, these findings also suggest that work nonself-
determined motivation may be more specifically associated with
consequences involving others (i.e., citizenship and interpersonal
deviant behaviours), rather than those usually originating from the
self (i.e., organisational involvement and commitment). In a sim-
ilar vein, previous research revealed that low levels of self-
determination may create a “passive set” such that individuals are
more likely to do the minimum amount of work required to obtain
desired rewards or avoid punishments (Pelletier et al., 1995). The
end result may be a corresponding lack of citizenship behaviour
(e.g., “always find fault with what other crew members are doing,”
“discouraging each other when someone is down”), and an in-
crease in deviant behaviours (e.g., “acting rudely toward someone,
making an ethnic, religious or racial remark”).
Together with the W–SDI, these are the two different ways of
computing the six types of motivation assessed with the WEIMS.
Researchers, as well as practitioners, may use either index depend-
ing on their objectives. However, one may argue that the use of
only one index (i.e., W–SDI and W–SDM vs. W–NSDM) may
lead to incomplete information. For example, they may fail to
indicate which particular type of motivation (e.g., IM vs. INTRO
vs. AMO) is the best indicator of various consequences. Further-
more, they may fail to identify changes occurring over time with
respect to the impact of the different types of motivation on
different criteria. For that reason, work motivation should still be
viewed as a multidimensional concept, with six different types of
motivation lying along a self-determination continuum (Deci &
Ryan, 1985). In fact, SDT’s original view of motivation was
supported by the present results with different types of motivation
being associated with a wide range of criteria (both positive and
negative; see Table 4).
Results pertaining to the validation of the WEIMS are also
consistent with the findings obtained with similar SDT-based
instruments used in other life domains (e.g., sports: Pelletier et al.,
1995; environment: Pelletier, Tuson, Green-Demers, Noels &
Beaton, 1998). This enables a high degree of consistency in
estimates of factor loadings, reliability, and intercorrelations as
well as content and criterion validity. In addition, the WEIMS’s
factorial structure was tested via a multisamples analysis by means
of structural equation modelling. The proposed pattern of relation-
ships was found significant in and invariant across, both military
and civilian samples. This finding of invariance suggests that the
factorial structure of the WEIMS seems rather stable within these
two distinct occupational groups. This further suggests that the
WEIMS can be used across different populations of workers with
minimum concern for sample specificity.
Table 5
Descriptive Statistics and Correlations (Cronbach’s alpha) Between the Two Forms of Work
Motivation and Related Consequences (Study 3)
Statistics Motivation Consequences
Variable M SD 1 2 3 4 5 6
Motivation
1. W–SDM 4.35 1.18 (.87) .31 .49 .53 .35 ?.16
2. W–NSDM 3.73 .92 (.72) .13 .12 ?.16 .18
Consequences
3. Involvement 4.27 .82 (.85) .66 .38 ?.12
4. Commitment 4.43 .90 (.90) .45 ?.07
5. Citizenship 5.12 .93 (.90) ?.27
6. Deviance 1.69 .53 (.84)
Note. N ? 192. rs ? .16 are significant at p ? .05. W–SDM ? Work Self-Determined Motivation;
W–NSDM ? Work Nonself-Determined Motivation.
Table 6
Standardized Parameter Estimates (?) and Adjusted R
2
Values for Each Prediction Based on the
Two Work Motivational Forms (Study 3)
Motivation Consequences
Variable W–SDM W–NSDM Involvement Commitment Citizenship Deviance
Motivation forms
W–SDM — — .49 .54 .44 ?.24
W–NSDM — — ns ns ?.29 .25
R
2
.17 ns .23 .27 .19 .07
Note. N ? 192. All parameter estimates are significant at p ? .001. W–SDM ? Work Self-Determined
Motivation; W–NSDM ? Work Nonself-Determined Motivation; ns ? not significant.
222
TREMBLAY ET AL.
Nonetheless, some results pertaining to the quasi-simplex pat-
tern of relationships for Study 1 yielded unexpected results. In-
trojection was highly associated with one type of self-determined
motivation, namely integration. Regarding this, a posteriori de-
scriptive statistics revealed that CF members displayed higher
levels of introjection than their civilian counterparts. Also, a pos-
teriori Pearson correlations were performed on the WEIMS’s sub-
scales of Study 3. Results revealed that the pattern of relationships
was similar to the one displayed in the original French validation.
That is, in the civilian sample, introjection was less strongly
associated with integration than in the military sample. It was more
strongly related to the other two nonself-determined types of
motivation, namely external regulation and amotivation. This may
reflect different employment conditions and/or a byproduct of the
organisational culture (e.g., Masi & Cooke, 2000; Trice & Beyer,
1993; Wilkins, 1989).
Future Research
Future research should investigate how personal characteristics
may lead to different motivational orientations. Sheldon and Elliot
(1998) pointed out that because self-determined goals originate
from personal values, and because they are viewed as the mech-
anism by which values transfer into action (Latham & Pinder,
2005), they arguably have an advantage over those that are exter-
nally forced (Deci & Ryan, 1991). Thusly they are likely to garner
the highest levels of effort and persistence.
During the last two decades, there has also been a shift in
organisations toward more group-based workforce (Ambrose &
Kulik, 1999). As they continue to move toward larger group-based
systems, research on work motivation is increasingly important
and should focus on its applicability to teams and team effective-
ness (e.g., Ellemers, de Gilder, & Haslam, 2004; Erez, Kleinbeck,
& Thierry, 2001). That is, there may be substantial processes
affecting teams that do not arise when the focus is on the individ-
ual (e.g., ways in which team members motivate and demotivate
one another; Locke & Latham, 2004).
Another topic for future research is the pattern of interrelation-
ships between the work outcome variables. Consequently, if the
goal is to understand employees’ behaviours, as well as predicting
them, one also cannot neglect measuring important mediating (and
potentially moderating) variables capturing the essence of such
phenomenon (Latham & Pinder, 2005).
As a last remark, research is needed to not only measure
work-related criteria via self-reports, but in addition to examine the
usefulness of the WEIMS in predicting actual behaviours, based on
previous evaluation of employee motivational orientation, whether
at the uni-, binary-, or multidimensional level of analysis. Given
that work motivation is a significant determinant of activity per-
sistence (Deci & Ryan, 1985), self-determined forms of work
motivation may be positively associated with work perseverance
(and negatively with voluntary turnover). Researchers ought to use
multiple methods, for instance self-reports, observation tech-
niques, and/or objective criteria.
Limitations
The limitations of the present research include the use of self-
reports and a cross-sectional design. Regarding the use of self-
reports, common-method variance may have occurred, leading to
spurious relationships between work motivation and its correlates.
A related limitation is the use of self-reports is the issue of social
desirability. For instance, individuals may have underreported
deviant behaviours because they did not want to admit to having
engaged in such socially undesirable actions. This type of mea-
surement error may partially account for the low means and high
skewness obtained in this research with regard to the measurement
of amotivation, workplace deviance, and turnover intentions.
Progress in this domain could be reached by juxtaposing implicit
measures of work motivation to subjective ones (such as the
WEIMS) to predict actual behaviours. It is believed that the valid
measurement of conscious work motives, using the WEIMS, rep-
resents a critical first step toward this goal. As a result of using
multiple techniques, different samples, and a variety of work
settings, the issue of generalisability could also be somewhat
resolved. Finally, given the cross-sectional nature of the present
research, arguments cannot be made that a particular form (or type)
of motivation was causal with respect to specific work-related
criteria.
Conclusions
Although results of the present research provide support for the
applicability and validity of the WEIMS, additional studies will be
necessary to further establish the psychometric properties of the
scale. Notwithstanding the aforementioned limitations, the current
research presents the WEIMS as a reliable and valid work moti-
vation instrument in its own right, assessing six theoretically
driven motivational tendencies and offering multiple indexes,
which are worth using in future research within the field of
organisational psychology, more specifically on research pertain-
ing to work motivation.
It is our hope that the use of the WEIMS will contribute to a
better comprehension of the interaction between work character-
istics, employee motivation, and organisational functioning. That
is, the WEIMS should be viewed a useful tool in identifying how
general variables such as motivational profiles get applied to, and
are mediated by, task- and situationally specific variables, and how
they affect individual choices and overall organisation structuring.
Re´sume´
L’e´chelle de motivation intrinse`que et extrinse`que au travail
(EMIET) est une mesure a` 18 items de la motivation au travail
ayant pour fondation the´orique la the´orie d’autode´termination
(Deci & Ryan, 2000). La pre´sente recherche s’organisaient avait
deux objectifs. Premie`rement, l’applicabilite´ de l’EMIET a e´te´
teste´e dans diffe´rents milieux de travail. Deuxie`mement, sa
structure ainsi que ses proprie´te´s psychome´triques ont e´te´ mesu-
re´es. Deux e´chantillons de travailleurs (militaires : N ? 465;
civils : N ? 192) volontaires ont rempli les questionnaires. En
utilisant les indexes de l’EMIET 3 (respectivement, l’index
d’autode´termination au travail, de motivation au travail autode´ter-
mine´e et non autode´termine´e), les re´sultats des analyses de re´gres-
sion ont appuye´ sa capacite´ pre´dictive de crite`res positifs et ne´gat-
ifs au travail. Les re´sultats ont aussi montre´ l’ade´quation de sa
validite´ de construit et de sa consistance interne. Sa structure
factorielle e´tait aussi constante a` travers les groupes. Finalement,
223
SPECIAL SECTION: WORK MOTIVATION: WEIMS
son patron quasi-simplex et ses relations avec des corre´lats psy-
chologiques sont davantage venus appuyer le continuum
d’autode´termination. En somme, ces re´sultats fournissent des ap-
puis pour l’applicabilite´, la fide´lite´ et la validite´ de l’EMIET en
milieu de travail.
Mots-cle´s : motivation au travail, the´orie d’autode´termination,
validation d’e´chelle
References
Allen, N. J., & Meyer, J. P. (1990). The measurement and antecedents of
affective, continuance, and normative commitment to the organization.
Journal of Occupational and Organizational Psychology, 63, 1–18.
Ambrose, M. L., & Kulik, C. T. (1999). Old friends, new faces: Motivation
research in the 1990s. Journal of Management, 25, 231–292.
Anderson, J., & Gerbing, D. W. (1988). Structural equations modeling in
practice: A review and recommended two-step approach. Psychological
Bulletin, 103, 411–423.
Baard, P. P., Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (2004). The relation of intrinsic
need satisfaction to performance and well-being in two work settings.
Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 34, 2045–2068.
Bagozzi, R. P., Yi, Y., & Phillips, L. W. (1991). Assessing construct
validity in organizational research. Administrative Science Quarterly,
36, 421–458.
Baron, R. A. (1991). Motivation in work settings: Reflections on the core
of organizational research. Motivation and Emotion, 15, 1–8.
Bartone, P. T., Ursano, R. J., Wright, K. M., & Ingraham, L. H. (1989). The
impact of a military air disaster on the health of assistance workers: A
prospective study. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 177, 317–
327.
Bennett, R. J., & Robinson, S. L. (2000). Development of a Measure of
Workplace Deviance. Journal of Applied Psychology, 85, 349–360.
Bentler, P. M. (2005). EQS 6.1 for Windows. Multivariate Software.
Bernard, J. (2001). Les facteurs explicatifs de la re´tention du personnel
d’officiers au sein du Commandement Ae´rien des Forces Canadiennes.
Ottawa : Defence Research and Development Canada.
Blais, M. R., Lachance, L., Vallerand, R. J., Brie`re, N. M., & Riddle, A. S.
(1993). The Blais Inventory of Work Motivation [French]. Revue Que´-
be´coise de Psychologie, 14, 185–215.
Blanton, H., & Jaccard, J. (2006). Arbitrary metrics in psychology. Amer-
ican Psychologist, 61, 27–41.
Bollen, K. A. (1989). Structural equations with latent variables. New
York: Wiley.
Byrne, B. M. (1994). Structural equation modeling with EQS and EQS/
Windows: Basic concepts, applications, and programming. London:
Sage.
Deci, E. L., Connell, J. P., & Ryan, R. M. (1989). Self-determination in a
work organization. Journal of Applied Psychology, 74, 580–590.
Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (1985). Intrinsic motivation and self-
determination in human behavior. New York: Plenum.
Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (1991). A motivational approach to integration
of self: Integration in personality. In R. Dienstbier (Ed.), Nebraska
Symposium on Motivation: Perspectives on motivation (pp. 237–288).
Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press.
Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (2000). The “what” and “why” of goal pursuits:
Human needs and the self-determination of behavior. Psychological
Inquiry, 11, 227–268.
Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (2002). Handbook of self-determination
research. Rochester, NY: University of Rochester Press.
Eisenberg, R., Huntington, R., Hutchison, S., & Sowa, D. (1986). Per-
ceived organizational support. Journal of Applied Psychology, 71, 200–
207.
Ellemers, N., de Gilder, D., & Haslam, S. A. (2004). Motivating individ-
uals and groups at work: A social identity perspective on leadership and
group performance. Academy of Management Review, 29, 459–478.
Ellemers, N., de Gilder, D., & van den Heuvel, H. (1998). Career-oriented
versus team-oriented commitment and behavior at work. Journal of
Applied Psychology, 83, 717–730.
Erez, M., Kleinbeck, U., & Thierry, H. (2001). Work motivation in the
context of a globalizing economy. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Fortier, M. S., Vallerand, R. J., & Guay, F. (1995). Academic motivation
and school performance: Toward a structural model. Contemporary
Educational Psychology, 20, 257–274.
Gagne´, M. (2003). The role of autonomy support and autonomy orientation
in the engagement of prosocial behavior. Motivation and Emotion, 27,
199–223.
Gagne´, M., & Deci, E. L. (2005). Self-determination theory and work
motivation. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 26, 331–362.
Gagne´ M., & Forest, J. (2008). The study of compensation systems through
the lens of self-determination theory: Reconciling 35 years of debate.
Canadian Psychology, 49, 225–232.
Green-Demers, I., Pelletier, L. G., & Me´nard, S. (1997). The impact of
behavioural difficulty on the saliency of the association between self-
determined motivation and environmental behaviours. Canadian Jour-
nal of Behavioural Science, 29, 157–166.
Greenwald, A. G., & Banaji, M. R. (1995). Implicit social cognition:
Attitudes, self-esteem, and stereotypes. Psychological Review, 102,
4–27.
Guay, F., Blais, M. R., Vallerand, R. J., & Pelletier, L. G. (1999). The
Global Motivation Scale. Unpublished manuscript, Universite´ duq Ue´-
bec a` Montre´al.
Hackman, J. R., & Oldham, G. R. (1975). Development of the Job Diag-
nostic Survey. Journal of Applied Psychology, 60, 159–170.
Houkes, I., Janssen, P. P. M., de Jonge, J., & Nijhuis, F. J. N. (2001).
Specific relationships between work characteristics and intrinsic work
motivation, burnout and turnover intentions: A multi-sample analysis.
European Journal of Work and Organizational Psychology, 10, 1–23.
Houkes, I., Jassen, P. P. M., de Jonge, J., & Bakker, A. B. (2003). Specific
determinants of intrinsic work motivation, emotional exhaustion and
turnover intention: A multisample longitudinal study. Journal of Occu-
pational and Organizational Psychology, 76, 427–450.
James, L. R., & Mazerolle, M. D. (2002). Personality in work organiza-
tions: An integrative approach. Beverly Hills, CA: Sage.
Johnson, R. E., & Steinman, L. (2009). Use of implicit measures for
organisational research: An empirical example. Canadian Journal of
Behavioural Science, 41, 202–212.
Jo¨reskog, K. G., & So¨rbom, D. (1989). LISREL 7: User’s reference guide.
Lincolnwood, IL: Scientific Software International.
Kanfer, R., Chen, G., & Pritchard, R. D. (2008). Work motivation: Forging
new perspectives and directions in the post-millennium. In R. Kanfer, G.
Chen, & R. D. Pritchard (Eds.), Work motivation: Past, present, and
future (pp. 601–631). New York: Taylor & Francis.
Kanfer, R., & Heggestad, E. D. (1997). Motivational traits and skills: A
person-centered approach to work motivation. Research in Organiza-
tional Behavior, 19, 1–56.
Kehr, H. M. (2004). Integrating implicit motives, explicit motives, and
perceived abilities: The compensatory model of work motivation and
volition. Academy of Management Review, 29, 479–499.
Kenny, D. A., & Kasby, D. A. (1992). Analysis of multi trait–multi method
matrix by confirmatory factor analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 112,
165–172.
Kline, R. B. (1998). Principles and practice of structural equation mod-
eling. New York: Guilford.
Landy, F. J., & Guion, R. M. (1970). Development of scales for the
measurement of work motivation. Organizational Behavior and Human
Performance, 5, 93–103.
Latham, G. P., & Pinder, C. C. (2005). Work motivation theory and
224
TREMBLAY ET AL.
research at the dawn of the twenty-first century. Annual Review of
Psychology, 56, 485–516.
Locke, E. A., & Latham, G. P. (2004). What should we do about motiva-
tion theory? Six recommendations for the twenty-first century. Academy
of Management Review, 28, 388–403.
Masi, R. J., & Cooke, R. A. (2000). Effects of transformational leadership
on subordinate motivation, empowering norms, and organizational pro-
ductivity. International Journal of Organizational Analysis, 8, 16–47.
Meyer, J. P., & Allen, N. J. (1991). A three-component conceptualization
of organizational commitment. Human Resource Management Review,
1, 61–89.
Meyer, J. P., Becker, T. E., & Vandenberghe, C. (2004). Employee
commitment and motivation: A conceptual analysis and integrative
model. Journal of Applied Psychology, 89, 91–1007.
Meyer, J. P., & Gagne´, M. (2008). Employee engagement from a self-
determination theory perspective. Industrial and Organizational Psy-
chology, 1, 60–62.
Miner, J. B. (2002). The role motivation theories of organizational lead-
ership. In F. J. Yammarino & B. J. Avolio (Eds.), Transformational and
charismatic leadership: The road ahead. New York: Elsevier.
Murray, H. A. (1943). Thematic Apperception Test manual. Cambridge,
MA: Harvard University Press.
Pelletier, L. G., Dion, S. C., Slovinec-D’Angelo, M., & Reid, R. (2004).
Why do you regulate what you eat? Relationship between forms of
regulation, eating behaviors, sustained dietary behavior change, and
psychological adjustment. Motivation and Emotion, 28, 245–277.
Pelletier, L. G., Tuson, K. M., Fortier, M. S., Vallerand, R. J., Brie`re,
N. M., & Blais, M. R. (1995). Toward a new measure of intrinsic
motivation, extrinsic motivation, and amotivation in sports: The Sport
Motivation Scale (SMS). Journal of Sports and Exercise Psychology, 17,
35–53.
Pelletier, L. G., Tuson, K. M., Green-Demers, I., Noels, K., & Beaton,
A. M. (1998). Why are you doing things for the environment? The
Motivation Towards the Environment Scale (MTES). Journal of Applied
Social Psychology, 28, 437–468.
Pelletier, L. G., Vallerand, R. J., Blais, M. R., Brie`re, N. M., & Green-
Demers, I. (1996). Construction and validation of the Leisure Motivation
Scale [French]. Loisir et Socie´te´, 19, 559–585.
Pinder, C. C. (1998). Work motivation in organizational behavior. Upper
Saddle River, NJ: Prentice Hall.
Ployhart, R. E. (2008). The measurement and analysis of motivation. In R.
Kanfer, G. Chen, & R. D. Pritchard (Eds.), Work motivation: Past,
present, and future (pp. 17–61). New York: Taylor & Francis.
Ployhart, R. E., Schneider, B., & Schmitt, N. (2006). Staffing organiza-
tions: Contemporaty practice and theory. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Podsakoff, P. M., Ahearne, M., & MacKenzie, S. B. (1997). Organizational
citizenship behavior and the quantity and quality of work group perfor-
mance. Journal of Applied Psychology, 82, 262–270.
Quast, H.-H., & Kleinbeck, U. (1990). Motivational determinants of ab-
sence behavior. In U. Kleinbeck, H.-H. Quast, H. Thierry, & H. Ha¨cker
(Eds.), Work motivation (pp. 157–167). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Roth, P. L., Huffcutt, A. I., & Bobko, P. (2003). Ethnic group differences
in measures of job performance: A new meta-analysis. Journal of
Applied Psychology, 88, 694–706.
Ryan, R. M. (1995). Psychological needs and the facilitation of integrative
processes. Journal of Personality, 63, 397–427.
Ryan, R. M., & Connell, J. P. (1989). Perceived locus of causality and
internalization. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 57, 749–
761.
Ryan, R. M., & Deci, E. L. (2000). Self-determination theory and the
facilitation of intrinsic motivation, social development, and well-being.
American Psychologist, 55, 68–78.
Ryan, R. M., & Deci, E. L. (2002). Overview of self-determination theory:
An organismic dialectical perspective. In E. L. Deci, & R. M. Ryan
(Eds.), Handbook of self-determination research (pp. 3–33). Rochester,
NY: University of Rochester Press.
Sheldon, K. M., & Elliot, A. J. (1998). Not all personal goals are personal:
Comparing autonomous and controlled reasons for goals as predictors of
effect and attainment. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 24,
546–557.
Steers, R. M., Mowday, R. T., & Shapiro, D. L. (2004). The future of work
motivation theory. Academy of Management Review, 29, 379–387.
Tabachnick, B. G., & Fidell, L. S. (2001). Using multivariate statistics.
Boston: Allyn & Bacon.
Thierry, H. (1990). Intrinsic motivation reconsidered. In U. Kleinbeck,
H.-H. Quast, H. Thierry, & H. Hacker (Eds.), Work motivation: Series in
applied psychology (pp. 67–82). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Trice, H. M., & Beyer, J. M. (1993). The culture of work organizations.
Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.
Tyler, T. R., & Blader, S. L. (2002). Terms of engagement: Why do people
invest themselves in work? Research on Managing Group and Teams, 4,
115–140.
Vallerand, R. J. (1989). Toward a methodology for the transcultural vali-
dation of psychological questionnaires: Implications for research in the
French language [French]. Canadian Psychology, 30, 662–680.
Vallerand, R. J. (1997). Toward a hierarchical model of intrinsic and
extrinsic motivation. In M. P. Zanna (Ed.), Advances in experimental
social psychology (pp. 271–360). San Diego, CA: Academic.
Vallerand, R. J., Blais, M. R., Brie`re, N. M., & Pelletier, L. G. (1989). On
the construction and validation of the French form of the Academic
Motivation Scale [French]. Canadian Journal of Behavioral Science, 21,
323–349.
Vallerand, R. J., & Ratelle, C. F. (2002). Intrinsic and extrinsic motivation:
A hierarchical model. In E. L. Deci & R. M. Ryan (Eds.), Handbook of
self-determination research (pp. 3–33). Rochester, NY: University of
Rochester Press.
Villeneuve, M., & Gingras, C. (1998). Report of the study of officer cadets’
perception of the organizational climate at the Royal Military College of
Canada. Kingston, Ontario: Defence Research and Development
Canada.
Warr, P. B., Cook, J., & Wall, T. D. (1979). Scales for the measurement of
some work attitudes and aspects of psychological well-being. Journal of
Occupational and Organizational Psychology, 52, 129–148.
Weege, J., Van Dick, R., Fisher, G. K., Wecking, C., & Moltzen, K.
(2006). Work motivation, organizational identification and well-being in
call centre work. Work and Stress, 20, 60–83.
Wilkins, A. L. (1989). Developing corporate character: How to success-
fully change an organization without destroying it. San Francisco:
Jossey-Bass.
Zuckerman, M., Porac, J., Lathin, D., Smith, R., & Deci, E. L. (1978). On
the importance of self-determination for intrinsically motivated behav-
ior. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 4, 443–446.
(Appendix follows)
225
SPECIAL SECTION: WORK MOTIVATION: WEIMS
Appendix A
Why Do You Do Your Work?
Using the scale below, please indicate to what extent each of the following items corresponds
to the reasons why you are presently involved in your work
Does not correspond
at all Corresponds moderately Corresponds exactly
1 2 3 4 5 6 7
1. Because this is the type of work I chose to do to attain a
certain lifestyle. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7
2. For the income it provides me. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7
3. I ask myself this question, I don’t seem to be able to
manage the important tasks related to this work. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7
4. Because I derive much pleasure from learning new
things. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7
5. Because it has become a fundamental part of who I am. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7
6. Because I want to succeed at this job, if not I would be
very ashamed of myself. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7
7. Because I chose this type of work to attain my career
goals. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7
8. For the satisfaction I experience from taking on
interesting challenges 1 2 3 4 5 6 7
9. Because it allows me to earn money. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7
10. Because it is part of the way in which I have chosen to
live my life. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7
11. Because I want to be very good at this work, otherwise I
would be very disappointed. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7
12. I don’t know why, we are provided with unrealistic
working conditions. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7
13. Because I want to be a “winner” in life. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7
14. Because it is the type of work I have chosen to attain
certain important objectives. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7
15. For the satisfaction I experience when I am successful at
doing difficult tasks. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7
16. Because this type of work provides me with security. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7
17. I don’t know, too much is expected of us. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7
18. Because this job is a part of my life. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7
Note. Intrinsic motivation ? 4,8,15; integrated regulation ? 5,10,18; identified regulation ? 1,7,14; introjected regula-
tion ? 6,11,13; external regulation ? 2,9,16; amotivation ? 3,12,17.
Received August 31, 2007
Revision received November 27, 2008
Accepted December 1, 2008 ?
226
TREMBLAY ET AL.

doc_520878975.pdf
 

Attachments

Back
Top